How the Visual Cliff Tested Babies' Depth Perception

Close up of smiling baby's face
Barbara Peacock / The Image Bank / Getty Images

A visual cliff involves an apparent, but not actual, drop from one surface to another. It was originally created to test babies' depth perception, and is formed by by connecting a transparent glass surface to an opaque patterned surface. The floor below has the same pattern as the opaque surface. This apparatus creates the visual illusion of a cliff while protecting the subject from injury.

History of the Visual Cliff

In order to investigate depth perception, psychologists E.J. Gibson and R.D. Walk developed the visual cliff test to use with human infants and animals. Gibson and Walk were interested in whether or not an infant's ability to perceive depth is a learned behavior or if it was, as they suspected, innate.

Earlier research revealed infants will respond to various depth cues even before they are able to crawl. Depth cues allow people to detect depth in a visual scene. These can include both monocular cues such as relative size and overlap, or binocular cues such as retinal disparity.

Gibson and Walk described their visual cliff apparatus as a large sheet of heavy Plexiglass supported a foot or more off the floor. Early versions of the experiments involved animal subjects such as turtles, goats, rats, lambs, kittens, dogs, pigs, and monkeys.

On one side of the glass, a high-contrast patterned fabric is pressed up against the underside to make the glass appear solid. The same material is laid on the floor below the glass, creating the visual illusion of a cliff. This allowed researchers to test infant perception while preserving the safety of their young subjects.

Visual Cliff Infant Test

In the test, a child is placed on the opaque end of the platform and the caregiver stands on the other side, with the clear surface. The assumption was that if a child had developed depth perception, they would be able to perceive the visual cliff and would either refuse or be reluctant to crawl to the caregiver. It was also assumed that infants who lacked depth perception would happily crawk to their caregivers without noticing the apparent drop.

Gibson and Walk concluded that the ability to perceive depth emerges sometime around the age that an infant begins to crawl. The fear of heights, they suggested, is something learned later in infancy as they gain experience with bumps, scrapes, and falls.

Understanding the Visual Cliff

Initially, psychologists believed the perception of the visual cliff was a matter of physical and visual maturity. Babies could see the difference by the age of eight months, while younger infants with less developed depth perception could not see the cliff.

Because six-month-old children could be enticed to wiggle across the visual edge, while 10-month-old children refused to cross the threshold, in 2013 it was assumed that the younger children had not yet developed depth perception while the older children had.

Later research published in 2014, however, has demonstrated that children as young as three months are able to perceive the visual cliff. When placed over the apparent "edge," their heart rates quicken, eyes widen, and breathing rates increase. So if these infants can perceive the visual cliff, why would they be willing to crawl off what appears to be a straight drop down?

The issue is that children of this age do not yet fully realize the consequence of going over this visual cliff is potentially falling. This realization only comes later when the child begins to crawl and gains real experience with taking tumbles.

The assumption has long been that avoidance of the visual cliff was related to a fear of heights, but recent research suggests infants avoid the drop-off because they sense they lack the physical skills to make the descent possible.

5 Sources
Verywell Mind uses only high-quality sources, including peer-reviewed studies, to support the facts within our articles. Read our editorial process to learn more about how we fact-check and keep our content accurate, reliable, and trustworthy.
  1. Gibson EJ, Walk RD. The "visual cliff". Sci Am. 1960;202:64-71.

  2. Rodkey EN. The visual cliff's forgotten menagerie: rats, goats, babies, and myth-making in the history of psychology. J Hist Behav Sci. 2015;51(2):113-40. doi:10.1002/jhbs.21712

  3. Kretch KS, Adolph KE. Cliff or step? Posture-specific learning at the edge of a drop-offChild Dev. 2013;84(1):226–240. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01842.x

  4. Dahl A, Campos JJ, Anderson DI, et al. The epigenesis of wariness of heightsPsychol Sci. 2013;24(7):1361–1367. doi:10.1177/0956797613476047

  5. Adolph KE, Kretch KS, LoBue V. Fear of heights in infantsCurr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(1):60–66. doi:10.1177/0963721413498895

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd
Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."